July 28, 2016
Written by Maximus Peperkamp, M.S. Behavioral Engineer
Dear Reader,
Here is my second response to “Verbal Behavior” by B.F.
Skinner (1957). The book contains two “Forewords”; one by J. Michael and another
by E. Vargas. This is my response to Vargas, who provided important historical
context. According to him Skinner “was not optimistic about the reception of
his analysis.” Skinner said in a letter to F.S. Keller “I feel hopeless about
convincing the linguists.”
This makes me think how very different I am from Skinner. I
would never write or say that I am hopeless about Sound Verbal Behavior (SVB). Also,
I am not trying to convince anyone. Convincing someone is characteristic of
Noxious Verbal Behavior (NVB). We can only engage in SVB if we have nothing to
prove. It doesn’t surprise me that Skinner would say this about what he
considered to be his most important book.
He wouldn’t have said this if he wasn’t trying to prove a
point. To his friend Keller, however, he wrote “What I am doing is applying
concepts I’ve worked out experimentally to this non-experimental (but
Empirical) field.” This second statement doesn’t sound pessimistic at all.
Skinner confidently states what he was doing. In SVB, communicators are
supportive of each other and thus between Skinner and Keller there can be SVB.
Linguists are not
supportive of behaviorists and behaviorists aren’t supportive of linguists
either and this lack of support sets the stage for NVB. Moreover, even
behaviorists (or linguists) among themselves aren’t supportive of each other.
Vargas writes that “Even behavioral scientists, including those calling
themselves “behaviorists” have not been convinced of the value of his analysis
of verbal behavior.”
The preceding sentence contains again that word: “convinced.”
We are not going to have SVB because
we are convinced. As long as we are trying to convince each other or believe to
be convinced, we will engage in NVB. Convincing each other or ourselves is not
part of SVB, as there is agreement instead of struggle between the speaker and
the listener.
According to Vargas “It seems to have been difficult to have accepted or to have understood two fundamental matters: that verbal behavior is shaped and
maintained by the same selection mechanisms that shape and maintain nonverbal
behavior, and that, concurrently, verbal behavior is distinguished by certain
characteristics that call for a separate analysis” (italics added). There is a
great difference between accepting
versus understanding these “two
fundamental matters.”
Acceptance will make understanding possible, but the absence
of acceptance will make understanding impossible. Also, of course, it is
possible to accept things without
understanding it, but it is impossible to understand things without accepting
it. The only exception to this is when there is to trust that the understanding
will eventually come.
If trust isn’t addressed first, learning isn’t possible and
understanding cannot arise. The real
issue of verbal behavior is not about
these “two fundamental matters”, but about the trusting situation which will
give rise to understanding. In other words, SVB must replace NVB before we can
learn anything. To the extent that this remains unacknowledged we keep making
it seem as if what we say is more
important than how we say it.
Interestingly, these “two fundamental matters” explain this.
Interestingly, these “two fundamental matters” explain this.
If “verbal behavior is shaped and maintained by the same
selection mechanisms that shape and maintain nonverbal behavior” then the how ofwhat we say “is distinguished by certain characteristics [SVB
&NVB] that call for a separate analysis.”
The two response classes SVB and NVB are a crucially important extension
of Skinner’s verbal behavior.
Vargas writes about the “selection by consequences and its
mediation by a verbal community.” He states that “Language, verbal practices
and the conditions that give them meaning, requires a culture—a verbal
community. The verbal community distinguishes the human organism.”
Another level of analysis than culture is needed to make sense
of language. We learn from our parents, family members and from the members of
the local community. Their way of
interacting directly shapes our
verbal behavior. If they engage in SVB, we learn to have SVB, but if they
engage with us in NVB, we will learn to have NVB.
Cultural differences, therefore, can be measured as a ratio of
SVB and NVB in each verbal episode. American culture has a higher rate of NVB
than Dutch culture. The SVB/NVB distinction will make our cultural differences tangible.
Our verbal community instructs us to achieve and maintain particular rates of
SVB and NVB. Thus, literacy involves much more than achieving “the prescribed
standards of verbal behavior.”
We would all benefit from low rates of SVB as behaving
verbally requires the behavior of others. When everyone only thinks of oneself,
the stage is set for NVB. Thus, the fact that “verbal behavior requires other
behavior” always reserves a place for SVB albeit a small one.
The less importance SVB has in any society, the more problems
there will be among its people. “Skinner treats verbal behavior as a natural
phenomenon, not mystical, but not reducible, either to the language of physics
or even biology. ” Verbal behavior should also not be reduced to NVB, as NVB makes the concept of social mediation
totally irrelevant.
That the NVB speaker is presumably always allowed to coerce the listener indicates a
hierarchical relationship in which verbal behavior will not be treated as a
natural phenomenon as the speaker is able to force his superstition on others.
Social mediation while speaking requires that the speaker and the listener are
equals and that each of the communicators can be experienced as the
speaker-as-own-listener.
Vargas states that “Behavior, both nonverbal and verbal, is
shaped and maintained by physical and biological worlds, but in addition to
these domains of phenomena, a social world shapes and maintains behavior.” He
follows in the footsteps of Skinner, who derived his theory of verbal behavior from
his laboratory work with nonverbal animals. The “experimental relations
discovered there” led to the interpretations, which formed the “basic
assertions of the functional relations between verbal behavior and other
behavioral and verbal events.”
Skinner’s interpretive work couldn’t lead to the distinction
between Sound Verbal Behavior (SVB) and Noxious Verbal Behavior (NVB). That
distinction would only become apparent after one had failed to speak and wasn’t
listened to. The SVB/NVB distinction could only emerge from struggle and after one
had investigated and understood why one’s speech didn’t have the effect one
wanted it to have and after one had desperately explored why one wasn’t able to
listen and understand.
Neither Skinner nor Vargas had to struggle with their speaking
and listening behavior to the extent that they had to became aware of two ways
of speaking that are experienced by the listener. Consequently, they mainly adhere
to the speaker’s and not the listener’s point of view.
If Skinner or Vargas had really wanted to provide “experimental
evidence for any of the propositions advanced” they would have had to explore
live conversations in which the effects of the speaker on the listener could be
verified. To explore the listener’s experience of the speaker would require the
listener to become a speaker and the speaker to become a listener. In other
words, this would require SVB.
In NVB experimental evidence cannot be obtained as speakers
and listeners are assigned to their roles due to hierarchical differences. It
is evident that SVB and NVB “are directly shaped” by the organisms “contact
with an immediate milieu,” but the experience of this process by the speaker
and the listener is the only way in which one can gather evidence.
Such experimental evidence requires there is turn-taking.
Such experimental evidence requires there is turn-taking.
As communicators explore and describe the nature of speaking
and listening while they are speaking
and listening, they agree that SVB is useful,
but NVB is useless communication.
Furthermore, it is only during SVB that communicators find that they are building
each other up, whereas in NVB they find that they are tearing each other down. These
are significant differences which are easily identifiable.
The aversiveness of the NVB speaker’s sound mainly elicits reflexive or respondent,
negative emotional behavior in the listener, while the comforting, soothing
sound of the SVB speaker’s voice evokes
operant, novel, emotional behavior in the listener. Only of SVB we can say “A
system, of contingent interdependent relations, arises from the interactions of
actions and events as they dynamically and reciprocally affect each other over
time.” NVB is not “a system, of
contingent interdependent relations.” In NVB, there are no “interactions of actions and events” which over time
“dynamically and reciprocally affect each other.”
In NVB communicators behave verbally, but they don’t reciprocate each other. NVB is mistaken
as communication, it is a one-way-street, which only goes from the speaker to
the listener. In NVB there is no turn-taking
in which the speaker can become the
listener and the listener can become
the speaker.
“Behavior of one organism determines the behavior of another,
but only because the other the second organism contacts directly the relation
of the first organism’s behavior to other events.” Vargas gives an example of
“direct” effects by describing behavior of a deer, who also hides and becomes
quiet after observing that marmosets suddenly scatter and hide as a predator
appears. Direct fearful responses map
onto NVB. SVB deals with indirect peaceful responses as “the organism may
also affect its world through the actions of other organism by virtue of
membership in a given verbal community of organisms.“
Vargas goes on to describe that “membership in a community may
also occur culturally.” He mentions “Amish, Buddhist, Catholic, Jewish, Muslim”
and “Protestant” and adds “capitalists, communists, fascists” and “socialist”,
but he completely ignores the important fact that in each of these verbal
communities there are people who have SVB and NVB. Thus, within each verbal
community there are two communities.
Each of these communities consequates differently “how members
behave verbally with respect to particular features of an immediate milieu or
with respect to the properties of verbal utterances.” In NVB, “consequating
behavior mediates” a different relationship “between what is said and what
occurs” than in SVB. During NVB “what occurs”, what happens nonverbally, is of
greater importance than “what is said”, what happens verbally. The emperor, who
orders his slave “to pass the grapes” doesn’t need to speak and simply waves or points his hand.
Obeying demanding nonverbal behavior equals survival; someone
who is inferior consequates gestures made by a superior. This is unlike verbal
behavior, which “exerts no direct mechanical or chemical force on the physical
properties of its immediate world.” Whether the other person is needed “to pass the grapes” or is forced “to pass the grapes” makes a big difference.
In both cases the slave will “pass the
grapes”, but in SVB it will be because of verbal behavior, while in NVB it will
be because of nonverbal behavior.
The “four-term contingency” only makes sense in environment
which is free of aversive stimulation in which SVB can occur. “Initial
stimulating events (the presence of the grapes, an audience, etc.), the
verbalizing action (for example, gesturing or speaking or writing “please pass
the grapes”), the consequating action that mediates the verbalization (passing
the grapes), and the consequence (obtaining the grapes).” The NVB, coercive,
nonverbal emperor instills negative emotions in the slave, who is negatively
reinforced for his or her nonverbal obedience.
Vargas also gives an example of a child throwing a temper
tantrum to illustrate how such behavior can easily become the manner in which
the child “communicates his desires.” He states “The teacher and therapist deal
constantly with and attempt to correct, these improperly shaped
verbalizations.” I happen to be a teacher and
a therapist and I know a thing or two about “Such “speech” – denoted “mands” by
Skinner.” My whole point is that unscientific NVB, in which we constantly de-“mand” each other’s
attention, consists of “improperly shaped verbalizations.”
Vargas writes that “the institutionalized effort to augment
the control of the immediate world over what is said about it, so more effective control can ensue over that world,
is now called “science”” (italics added). If we really are going to make a
scientific, “institutionalized effort” over what we say about our world,
so we can increase control over that world by our way of talking, we can’t
keep on demanding with our biased, forceful NVB and we need to learn how to have
SVB.
I disagree with Vargas, who believes “Once verbal behavior
begins to have effects on the physical and biological world about it through
the behavior of others, it can have effects independent of that world. It
becomes sufficient only to affect others.” It is not sufficient (for speakers) to “only to affect others” (listeners)
as how we affect others determines whether
our verbal behavior is sufficient or not.
NVB is insufficient as “verbal actions” that act as coercive
stimuli “in sequences of “verbalizations” independent of their relation to the
world about them.” Echoing these verbalizations “easily leads to superstitious
behavior, as actions to verbal utterances become confounded with the events and
objects to which the utterances refer.” The philosophical implication of SVB is
not the verbalizer’s proper tacting,
but his or her ability to properly affect and regulate the body of the mediator
with the sound of his or her voice. Modern mutually reinforcing behavioral
control will replace our outdated NVB.
Vargas provides another example to illustrate that “The
controls over a verbal action define its meaning.” He explains the multiple
meanings of the statement “Polly wants some spinach.” Each of his explanations concerns
some speaker. The fact that Vargas, but also Skinner, took a speaker’s
perspective, isn’t altered “if a young child named “Polly” says it, if Polly’s mother says
it to Polly’s father who is sitting next to Polly at the dinner table, if an
actor reads it from a script while
addressing another actor, if a parrot mimics it, or a reader reads it.”
This strict speaker’s perspective also continues in the
explanation “The young child named Polly may have been hungry when she stated that
she wanted more spinach.” Regardless of whether she “may have been feeding it
surreptitiously to her dog both for the dog’s sake and to please her parents,”
in each case it was the speaker, who said “Polly wants some spinach.” Vargas only
pays lip-service to the listener’s perspective when he writes “Listeners and
readers tend to infer these controls by observing the circumstances (or
learning of these circumstances) under which a person says or writes what is said
or written and by taking into account what they know of the person.”
Even when the listener is mentioned, it is to observe rather than to listen to what the almighty superior speaker
is saying. This reference to visual (nonverbal) stimuli (observing the
circumstances) instead of auditory (verbal) stimuli seems justified as the
“Listeners and readers” are simultaneously addressed. This mixing up of
“Listeners and readers” weakens any reference to the listener’s “control over a
verbal action.” A listener’s perspective is only mentioned as an afterthought,
but, even then, the speaker is still considered to be more important than the
listener. “And the speaker attempts to help the listener understand by
indicating the controls under which he makes his statement.” Speakers who
engage in NVB are not helping the
listener.
Dear Reader,
Dear Reader, here is the second part of my response to Vargas' Foreword to Skinner's Verbal Behavior (1957).I teach that the listener can help the speaker by letting him or her know how his or her voice is affecting him or her. If the listeners experience the speaker’s voice as an aversive stimulus, as is the case of NVB, the speaker can explain things until he or she is blue, but his or her “attempts to help the listener understand” are simply not working.
Dear Reader,
Dear Reader, here is the second part of my response to Vargas' Foreword to Skinner's Verbal Behavior (1957).I teach that the listener can help the speaker by letting him or her know how his or her voice is affecting him or her. If the listeners experience the speaker’s voice as an aversive stimulus, as is the case of NVB, the speaker can explain things until he or she is blue, but his or her “attempts to help the listener understand” are simply not working.
Observing is not the same as listening. Speakers get verbally fixated if they make more
verbal statements to explain a point. “It is going to rain” changes the meaning
for the listener if the speaker says, “ I heard on the radio that it is going
to rain”, “I just looked out the door and it is going to rain”, “ A few drops
hit the window so it is going to rain”,
“Let’s have a picnic since the radio said that there is no change that it is
going to rain.” It is not only the speaker who controls the verbal action. May
be the speaker can ask how the listener is feeling?
“Making the listener behave more effectively by coming in
contact with the circumstances that control the speaker’s behavior” might actually
require that the speaker stops talking and invites the listener to do some of
the talking. Only in SVB “The speaker is reinforced by the more accurate
behavior on the listener’s part.” In NVB it is always the attention-demanding speaker
who raises the issue of “more accurate behavior on the listener’s part,” but in
SVB “accurate behavior on the listener’s part” is a function of the listener’s
ability to speak.
It is only in NVB that the lack of listening keeps being brought
up as the listener is neither allowed to and often not even supposed to speak.
The NVB speaker may be able to force
the listener to listen to him or to her and he or she may be able to coerce the listener to do as he or she
says, but he or she will always demand
“more accurate behavior on the listener’s part” and never feel positively
reinforced. “The hunt for these controlling variables is actively pursued in
the attempt to become a more effective partner in the verbal transaction.” Those
who engage in SVB are not trying to “become a more effective partner.”
During SVB the controlling variables of our vocal verbal
behavior are apparent and there is no need to hunt for them. Moreover, in SVB,
the whole idea of becoming a more effective partner doesn’t even arise. All
such problem-perpetuating talk is part and parcel of NVB, in which the speaker presumably
is more important than the listener.
It was the NVB community who “set in motion efforts to contact
the contingencies controlling the verbalizer’s behavior – whether spoken,
written or gestured.” The SVB community, on the other hand, never put any
effort in contacting the contingencies controlling the verbalizer’s behavior. It effortlessly discovered the contingencies
controlling the listener’s behavior.
Discovery of the speaker-as-own-listener informs the speaker in ways which the
listener who is not speaker is incapable of. In other words, the “more accurate
behavior of the listener’s part” refers to the listener who is also simultaneously
the speaker.
Vargas gets very close, but not close enough, as he reads
Skinner and “comes under control of what impelled the writer to write.” He writes “In deconstructing a text the reader
understands its meaning only by understanding the controls over his or her own
reading behavior, and where they overlap with those of the writer.” But,
reading, of course, is not the same as listening and writing is not the same as
speaking.
It is not a matter of understanding, but of experiencing the
controls over our own listening behavior, which will make us listeners realize
where they overlap with those of the speaker. Attunement, rather than
understanding, will join our speaking and listening behavior.
Vargas, with his speaker’s perspective, states that “every act
of listening” is “an act of speaking,” but every act of speaking is not an act
of listening during NVB. This is only the case in SVB. Thus, the “Future
analysis of Skinner’s system of verbal relations” depends on the development of
the SVB community. The NVB community has hindered exploration of verbal
behavior and has maintained its many paradoxes.
“The overemphasized, even artificial, distinction between the
localities called “speaker” and “listener,” is a product of NVB, which is based
on the separation of the speaker and the listener by the speaker. Vargas seems
to refer to SVB and NVB when he states “At certain points of the flow of verbal
behavior [SVB], certain controls are in place and others are not [NVB], and
these controls may shift place with others, or in the sequence of verbal
actions such controls may exert their effects at certain points of the sequence
and not others.”
Only in SVB speakers and listeners can experience and
therefore wholeheartedly agree that “it makes little difference for the verbal
relations involved whether verbal behavior is taking place between two loci or
within one.” Close reading of someone who has extensively studied Verbal
Behavior lays bare why radical behaviorists and non-behaviorists alike,
“continually trip” over “paradoxes and difficulties.”
To the extent that they accepted NVB as simply “a heritage”
from their verbal community, behaviorists were unable to promote “operant
phenomena”. My writing is to enhance with SVB the dissemination of radical
behaviorism and behaviorology. I totally agree “the analysis of any further
work in verbal behavior will rest on the foundation” that Skinner provides in
Verbal Behavior. The distinction between
SVB and NVB was driven by the selection mechanisms that operate in my life.
The more I came to know about the SVB/NVB distinction the more
my SVB has been enhanced and the better I have become capable of teaching this
to others. Behaviorism and behaviorology have enhanced my knowledge of this
distinction and have made me successful in my work as a teacher and as therapist.
I am grateful to the behaviorist
community and look forward to talking with behaviorists and with
behaviorologists about my extension of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior.