August 10, 2015
Written
by Maximus Peperkamp, M.S. Verbal Engineer
Dear Reader,
This writing is my tenth response to “Talker-specific learning in
speech perception” by Nygaard and Pisoni (1998). "Serial recall of spoken word lists produced by multiple talkers was
poorer than recall of lists produced by a single talker; but the result was
found only in the primacy portion of the serial recall curve.” These results
need to be analyzed in terms of whether the speaker produced Sound Verbal
Behavior (SVB) or Noxious Verbal Behavior (NVB) and positively influenced
the listener with an appetitive sounding voice or an negatively influenced the
listener with an aversive sounding voice.
“The primacy portion of the serial
curve” is hypothesized to be absent in SVB and is believed to be mainly be a function of NVB. It was suggested that "variation in a talker’s voice from word to word
in a list competes for processing resources in the recall task.” This
interpretation doesn’t answer the question why this “competition for processing
resources” occurs. The SVB/NVB
distinction, however, makes us realize that this “competition” occurs only due
to NVB.
In SVB the verbal and nonverbal expressions of the speaker are aligned,
but in NVB they are two different messages as they are
disjointed. Moreover, in SVB the speaker is his or her own listener. This
always positively effects the feelings of the listener, but in NVB the speaker is not
listening to him or herself, which always negatively effects the listener. What is recalled by the listener from an aversive-sounding
NVB speaker is mainly that he or she sounds aversive.
Stated differently,
because lexical information dominates and hinders the linguistic analysis of
what the speaker says, the listener who listens to a NVB speaker is believed to
remember less than the listener who listens to a SVB speaker. Whether it is possible
or not, the listener who listens to a NVB speaker will try to move away from the
aversive stimulation of the speaker and consequently remember less of the lexical information.
The “analysis of talker information
during a memory task appears to be both time- and resource-demanding,” but only when
the listener is dealing with a NVB speaker. Reasoned from the SVB/NVB
distinction, we find that it is not the “talker variability” which “increases
the capacity demands of the working memory system”, but it is SVB which
increases this capacity and, by contrast, it is NVB, which decreases this working memory capacity.
The researchers noted that
recall is also affected by presentation rates. They don’t mention that
these, in turn, are determined by the kind of vocal verbal behavior of the
speakers, that is, by their SVB or NVB. A SVB speaker's speech episode contains more instances of SVB than NVB, while a NVB
speaker's presentation contains more NVB instances than SVB
instances.
The SVB presentation occurs at more relaxed pace and slower rate than the anxiety and stress provoking NVB presentation. Leaving out the influence of the talker’s voice on the listener, the
researchers overlook what may be the most important independent variable, which
is unspecified in the catch-all-phrase “talker variability.”
Authors unaware of the SVB/NVB distinction will maintain
‘mentalistic’ definitions, which are useless in any behavioral account. 'Talker variability' is a useless term if it doesn't address SVB and NVB. “This
interaction between presentation rate and serial recall for the multiple- and
single-talker word lists suggests that at fast presentation rates, when processing
is constrained by time, talker variability affects both the perceptual encoding
and the rehearsal of items in the serial recall task” (words underline
by me).
To consider the influence of the speaker’s sound, we should
do away with constructs that represent verbal bias. Conclusions are drawn which prevent us from finding out what is happening. “At slower presentation rates, when listeners have more time and
resources to encode and rehearse talker information, they are able to use that information
to aid them in the encoding of item and order information.” With a
SVB speaker the listener is at ease and better able to pay attention
to what he or she is saying. The SVB/NVB distinction is a more parsimonious explanation than
inferences about “encoding” and “rehearsing” of the “item and order
information”.
Based on my knowledge about SVB and NVB, I object to the researcher’s conclusion. “These
memory findings suggest that talker information may not be discarded in the
process of spoken word recognition, but rather is retained in memory along with
the more abstract, symbolic linguistic content of the utterance.” They seem to think that nebulous cognitive processes explain how “talker
information” is “retained in memory.”
What is left out by these authors is
the fact that the listener’s neural behavior is altered by the sound of the speaker's voice,
leading one listener to supposedly have better memory than the other. What
actually happens is that the body of the listener who ‘remembers’
what the speaker has said was positively affected by the tone of the speaker’s
voice. The stress that is produced by the NVB speaker always has an adverse
effect on memory.
If we don’t discard constructs
as “information”, we continue to misrepresent classical and operant conditioning
effects – in speakers and listeners – of
how the speaker sounds. Presumably “Talker-specific information
is retained in memory along with lexical information" and "this information
can facilitate listeners’ recognition memory.” SVB and NVB can be heard, but “talker-specific information” and “lexical
information”cannot.
Ironically, the
researchers, who found that “Words repeated in the same voice were recognized
better than words repeated in a different voice”, didn’t realize that fixation
on words, a characteristic of NVB, distracts them from paying attention to how this “same voice”
actually sounds.
No comments:
Post a Comment