February 17, 2015
Written by Maximus Peperkamp, M.S. Behavioral Engineer
Dear Reader,
The situation that sets the stage for spoken
communication is different from culture to culture. As individuals from
different cultures become exposed to unfamiliar contingencies, they soon realize what is punished in one culture is reinforced in another. In addition
to phylogenetic (caused by evolutionary processes) and ontogenetic (caused by processes occurring during an organism's life time) processes, B.F. Skinner pointed to culture as the
third component to be considered in explaining behavior.
This writer stimulates
the reader to consider the different sounds of these different cultures. German
sounds different from Hindi and Russian sounds different from
English. However, we can also find similarities
in how different cultures sound. This is where the distinction between Sound
and Noxious Verbal Behavior as two subsets of verbal behavior becomes
important. It doesn’t matter whether we speak Hindi or Russian, Sound Verbal
Behavior (SVB) refers to the verbal episodes in which the speaker controls the
behavior of the listener with positive reinforcement. In both
English and German, however, Noxious Verbal
Behavior (NVB) refers to the verbal episodes in which the speaker controls
the behavior of the listener with an aversive contingency. It is pragmatic to
consider SVB and NVB as languages from different cultures, because they too occur in different
environments. A hospital, in which nurses and doctors are trying to take care
of patients, is a different communication environment as a battlefield on
which soldiers are trying to kill each other. We have not been able
to see similarities in cultures, because our differences between SVB and
NVB inevitably got personal. That is, SVB and NVB explain human verbal behavior
at the level of the individual organism.
Although different topics, such as work, art, science, politics, family,
finances or sports, may characterize our spoken conversation, limited
interaction is possible when people don’t
speak the same language, that is, when they don’t sound the same. Swedish sounds don’t make any sense to a French
person who has never been exposed to Swedish sounds.
The same stimulus generalization principle that is involved in teaching a
child to say “dog” to a German shepherd as well as to a Chihuahua, is at work
in learning to speak a different language than one’s native language. In
learning to say “dog”, in the presence of relatively different-looking creatures, the child responds, however, to the
similarities. Thus, the child learns different concepts such as tool, food and transportation. Although a hammer looks very different than a saw, a hammer
and a saw are tacted as a “tool”, because of what we use them for. Likewise, the
common element class “as something
eatable” evokes our ability to discriminate apple, bread and candy as “food.”
However, "as research with nonverbal pigeons
has often demonstrated, verbal behavior is not necessarily involved in such
conceptual behavior" (Martin & Pear, 2007, p. 106). The principle of behavioral continuity is illustrated by
the fact that pigeons can easily be taught to respond to concepts such as
“fish” or “human.” Thus, when a child responds with “fish” upon hearing words
such as shark, sardine and whale or seeing pictures of these, this child has
learned “to emit the appropriate response to all the members of a stimulus
common-element class and does not emit that response to stimuli that do not
belong to that class” (Martin & Pear, 2007, p. 106).
As long as the distinction between SVB and NVB has not been made, we are not fully verbal,
that is, we are not showing the conceptual behavior that is necessary to call a spade a spade. Although we do have a vague notion of them as subsets of
verbal behavior, as evidenced by sayings such as: it is not what you say but how you say it, we often mistake NVB for SVB. The stimulus common-element class of NVB and SVB can only be correctly
discriminated if we listen to how we sound while we speak.
Voice I is the
common element in NVB and Voice II is the common element in SVB. Voice I is
called Voice I, because we will not be able to recognize Voice II without first identifying Voice I. Our mistake
to view NVB as SVB is based on our
unfulfilled need for peace, safety, stability and support, which functions as Establishing Operations for NVB. Unless we discriminate that the stimulus
classes that comprise our negative and our positive experiences (regardless of our phylogenetic,
ontogenetic or cultural heritage) always,
everywhere and under all circumstances sound
very different, we continue to mistake SVB for NVB and visa versa.
SVB and NVB are two important equivalence classes which can
be easily learned. What is needed is matching-to-sample. Only SVB
can reinforce SVB and only SVB can shed light on NVB. In a simple stimulus
equivalence experiment, a child may be taught during a couple trials to match
number 2 with a picture of two ducks on it. In a second number of trials, the
child is then reinforced for matching the picture of the two ducks with the
word “two.” During the third trial the child will be tested to see if it has
learned the equivalence class. If the child matches the word “two” with the
number 2 then “members of this equivalence class are functionally equivalent in
the sense that they all control the same behavior” (Martin & Pear, 2007, p.
106). We do more matching-to-sample training for NVB than SVB, because we don’t know SVB. We acquired equivalence classes, but don't see the negative consequences of stimulus
generalization. “When a new behavior becomes conditioned to one member of an
equivalence class, that behavior is likely to be controlled by other members of
the class without explicit training” (Martin & Pear, 2007, p. 108).
SVB and NVB are concepts which tell us about how we communicate. NVB has Voice I in common and SVB
has Voice II in common. However, what
we say in NVB pertains to the same equivalence class. What we say in SVB refers to an entirely different, for many new, way of communicating. As SVB is reinforced it will last longer.
Its newness is proportional to the extent that it lasts longer than the
previous time. Although most of us can recognize components of SVB, we have
not experienced repeated trials in which components came together and stayed together for a longer time. For this we must arrange
lab-like conditions in which we control for NVB. This will only be done if we see the need for it and the potential of it.
Generalization is said to fail when the child says “doggie” to a hairy
four-legged creature in the presence of a cat. Similarly, generalization fails
as long as we accept as normal the way of communicating to which we are used,
which we have come to expect and which we reinforce: NVB, in which the speaker
controls the behavior of the listener with an aversive contingency. It is
necessary that we teach discriminations and that we stop reinforcing aggressive or passive aggressive communication as
a way of communicating. Although NVB is verbal behavior, as it is
Noxious, listeners are aversively controlled by it and are bound to respond to
it with some form of counter-control, escape or avoidance.
Only SVB, in
which the speaker controls the listener with positive reinforcement, is to be
tacted as real communication. That is, SVB must
be differentiated from NVB, because it is a different subset of verbal
behavior. NVB may be better tacted as coercion, aggression, domination, exploitation,
intimidation, but not as
communication. A dog is not a cat.