July 10, 2015
Written
by Maximus Peperkamp, M.S. Verbal Engineer
Dear Reader,
This is my third response to the findings of Owren and
Rendall published in “An affect conditioning model of nonhuman primate vocal
signaling” (1997). The “conative”
function or natural tendency of primate vocal signaling is primarily affective,
because senders either induce positive or negative affective experiences in
receivers. Cognitive outcomes of signaling are secondary to as they are facilitated
by affective experiences that any given vocalization induces. Only “calls
which are primarily conative”, which result in a positive affective experience the receivers, could become “cognitive” in function. Those signals which resulted in negative affective experiences in the receivers led to an aversive kind of learning which, rather than stimulating verbal behavior, stimulated nonverbal behavior.
In humans, Noxious Verbal Behavior (NVB) is always
a function of negative affective experiences, such as a threat. The
vocalizations produced by the sender in NVB will make the receiver flee, fight or freeze. Such NVB vocalizations are not conducive to activation
of cognitive systems as they directly activate and dysregulate
subcortical systems which trigger avoidance and escape behavior instead of exploratory approach behavior.
Only a
sender’s Sound Verbal Behavior (SVB) has a directly regulating and stabilizing
effect on these subcortical systems. Only if circumstances are such that “the
influences that the senders are able to assert” are “beneficial to the
receiver” will the cognitive systems of the receiver be properly activated as a function of his or her positive affective experiences. If on the other hand, the circumstances are such that the sender’s vocalizations are “costly to the
receiver” or “detrimental to” his or her “overall fitness, natural selection
will favor adaptations that decrease those costs.”
To answer a question from a dominant NVB
sender, it is adaptive for the subordinate receiver to produce SVB. Thus, SVB
is a function of warding off the NVB sender and out of our increased ability to
become better at this and to be safe, our cognitive abilities are believed to have emerged. The less we were bothered by threats and the more we prolonged our positive affective states became, the more our
cognitive abilities could flourish.
“A number of relevant
principles have been identified in this regard, including constraints imposed by
the transmission of the environment in which calls are used (e.g. Brown, Gomez
& Waser, 1995) possible relationships among acoustic gradedness, and the
degree to which signals in other modalities compliment the acoustic event (e.g.
Green & Marler, 1997; Marler, 1975) and a general relationship between the
acoustic of calls and sender state (Morton, 1977, 1982). All of these effects are explained by the
Affect Conditioning Model (ACM), but they are even better explained by the
SVB/NVB distinction, which clarifies the relationship between form and
function, that is, “the bewildering variety of acoustic form and repertoire
design among primates” and human beings.
To the extent that we understand our own Pavlovian anticipatory affective reactions “in response to
upcoming events” we will be able to acknowledge that “affective responses
play a central role in such learning.” As long as we don’t realize that NVB continues to make us talk and write only in a NVB manner, we are unable to comprehend that
another way of conditioning, with SVB, would have led to totally different and more positive results.
The crucial role played by “acoustic cues to
individual identity in vocal communication processes” is well-established and “evidence
of either discrimination or explicit recognition of individuals is available
from several species” (e.g. Snowdon, 1986; Rendall, Rodman, Emond, 1996).
Although there is significant evidence that “Adult
primates, particularly females, make use of individually distinctive acoustic
cues in calls in coming to the aid of related individuals or unrelated allies
involved in agonistic social encounters” and primate behavior is generally
shaped by “kinship relationship and interaction histories (see Smuths, Cheney,
Seyfarth, Wrangham, & Struhsaker, 1987; Cheney & Seyfarth, 1990),
nobody is paying attention to the primate in the room: humans influence each
other with their sound. These researchers are trying to get the proposal accepted that
“primates produce affective vocalizations in conspecifics receivers, thereby
influencing the subsequent behavior of those animals,” but they will only succeed if we know about the SVB/NVB distinction.