April 16, 2015
Written by Maximus Peperkamp, M.S. Verbal Engineer
Dear Reader,
Sound Verbal Behavior (SVB) and Noxious Verbal Behavior
(NVB) alter our nervous system. These neural changes, which make us mediate
behavior differently, happen because of the functional effects of stimuli: our
voices. However, in SVB we are conscious of
our voice, but in NVB we remain unconscious
about our sound. Stated differently, in SVB we are overt about how we sound, but in NVB, we are covert about it. In other words, we will continue to have NVB as
long as we don’t talk about how we sound.
We must differentiate between respondent hearing and
operant hearing. During the preverbal stages of development, unconditioned
stimulus energies at the baby’s phono-receptors in his or her ears produced by
the mother’s voice, elicit unconscious hearing as raw sensations. This is unconditioned respondent neural hearing
behavior. Conditioned hearing happens
because other stimuli occur together with this unconditioned stimulus. After the
mother’s voice has become paired with food, attention and comfort, these stimuli elicit
conditioned hearing responses in the baby. And, the baby at some point starts to speak when the
mother’s voice is paired with words.
I will now turn to the covert and overt aspects of operant
hearing. Covert operant hearing can be a response which is evoked by someone’s
voice, by an overt antecedent stimulus, but it can also be a response to a
previous covert antecedent response. In the former, the listener is listening
to the speaker, but in the latter, the listener is not listening to the speaker, but to him or to herself. The
distinction is crucially important because during such covert hearing we
neither talk about what we have heard overtly nor covertly.
Although in overt
operant hearing we can talk about what we have heard overtly as well as
covertly, we usually don’t so. In most of our conversations private speech
is pushed out by and abandoned in favor of public speech. Thus, when we can only
talk about what we hear overtly, we are bound to engage in NVB. If, on the
other hand, we can talk about what we hear covertly, we are bound to have SVB and we will be listening better to each other overtly. Stated differently, we don’t and we can’t have SVB as long as what we say to ourselves covertly cannot
be expressed overtly. Furthermore, overt hearing can be improved by covert
hearing. That is, each time we turn our conversation to what we hear covertly,
our overt hearing becomes better.
Another way of saying this is covert
hearing includes overt hearing, but
overt hearing excludes covert
hearing. Everybody has heard the overt complaint that we should and
could do a better job at listening to each other, that is, by overt
hearing. However, this emphasis on overt hearing hasn’t changed our behavior,
because it excluded covert hearing.
The NVB speaker elicits overt hearing in the listener.
When the speaker, with his or her sound, controls the listener behavior with an
aversive contingency, the listener is prevented from covert hearing. When uni-directional overt ‘communication’ is demanded by the speaker,
the listener is pretty much forced to disconnect from his or her own covert private speech. Although this covert disconnect causes many overt
disruptions, we are conditioned by NVB to ignore and punish such effects. Indeed,
we have learned to punish and blame ourselves. Thus, private speech is alienated
from our public speech.
By reversing the split between our private speech and public speech,
we accomplish SVB. By moving away, while we speak, from overt hearing to covert
hearing and by making overt what we covertly hear, we can finally overtly hear
what we have been saying to ourselves covertly.
Skinner explained consciousness as “seeing that we are seeing” and called this “conscious seeing”
(1963). I suggest that consciousness is 'hearing that we are listening." Like seeing, hearing is made
possible by the operant contingencies of a verbal community, which conditions unconscious
hearing responses as well as what we say about what we hear. Initially, what we
hear elicits raw sensation, covert, unconscious hearing. From our verbal
community we learn to listen, talk, write and read; we become
conscious about what started out as an unconscious experience. Another way of
referring to this is that we transition from nonverbal to verbal
behavior.
Since we are born as nonverbal organisms, our listening
behavior develops first and forms the foundation from which we become verbal.
To ‘hear that we are listening’ and to ‘hear consciously’ requires that we pay
attention to our sound while we speak. Since we seldom do that and at best have
only experienced this momentarily and accidentally, we are mostly unconscious while we are
speaking. In most conversations speakers don’t pay attention to how
they sound, but they demand that others to pay attention to them.
When we speak, we produce sound. Vocal behavior preceded
vocal verbal behavior ontogenetically and phylogenetically. In other words,
vocal behavior goes back a much longer time into our evolutionary history than
our vocal verbal behavior. Our survival depended on hearing and responding to
the vocalizations from our conspecifics. There was no need to be conscious about raw
sensation vocal behavior, because it would instantly elicit the adaptive
autonomic response. It should be apparent from this example that our neural
behavior never produced our hearing
response, but always was and still is that hearing response. This
neurobiological fact is not changed by the arrival of language, due to which we
became capable of describing our reflexive vocal response and became conscious of
it. However, only to the extent that our descriptions are accurate can we
become conscious communicators. Stated differently, only our inaccurate
descriptions ignore the eliciting effects which are created by our raw
sensation vocal behavior.
Although we have been conditioned by inaccurate
descriptions of our vocal verbal behavior, that is, by NVB, our bodies haven’t changed that much and
respond quite naturally to accurate descriptions. Obviously, such
descriptions describe danger as dangerous and safety as safe. Danger is still
felt by most of us as danger and safety is still felt by us as safe. Once we have
the distinction correct between SVB and NVB, we recognize how much so-called
communication is still NVB, because it is based on threat and intimidation. Only
because we haven’t had ongoing SVB and because we keep having instances
of NVB, we consider the description of consciousness and the neural basis for
it, to be separate from consciousness itself.
No comments:
Post a Comment