November 15, 2015
Written by Maximus Peperkamp,
M.S. Verbal Engineer
Dear Student,
This is a second response to the paper “Seeing with ears:
Sightless human’s perception of dog bark provides a test for structural rules
in vocal communication” (2009) by C.Molnar,
P.Pongracz and A. Miklosi. Before I will further comment on this paper, I first
want to tell you about the wonderful day I had yesterday with my wife Bonnie.
We had our thirty year marriage anniversary. In the morning, we went for a nice
walk in the park. Then we went shopping and bought two honeysuckle plants,
which I planted in our garden. It was a day filled with joy and peace. I had a
wonderful skype conversation with Arturo, my behaviorist friend from Colombia.
It was so nice to share our happiness with him. After a lovely lunch and some
wine I took a blissful nap, while Bonnie was watching her favorite show on TV.
In the evening, I was
interviewed by Sue Hilderbrand. She has her own talk-show called “The Real
Issue” at KZFR, the local radio station. The interview went very well and my
message of Sound Verbal Behavior (SVB) had a big impact on her and on many
others. Ben, a friend of Sue, also joined the conversation and was explaining
in his own words what I was talking about. Jake, an environmentalist, who was to
be interviewed after me, also said many validating things about our need for
SVB. Sue, who is involved in local politics, said that she would definitely
have me in her show again. Bonnie was reading a book in bed when I came home.
It was cozy to join her. I also read a little, but soon I was overcome by
sleep. I slept well and long and had a beautiful dream about the history of my
knowledge. I was feeling so grateful that I almost started to cry, but I
didn’t. I slept much longer than usual as I was having these positive emotions.
My body feels rested and my
thoughts about the distinction between SVB and Noxious Verbal Behavior (NVB)
are clear. To my knowledge nobody has analyzed spoken communication in the way
I do. There are immense implications of my analysis. Sue, who interviewed me,
brought up the issue of fear for being open. I said that our fear is not for
SVB, but for NVB or rather, our fear signifies the absence of SVB. She agreed.
We talked about how SVB public speech causes SVB private speech and how such
SVB private speech allows us to recognize, avoid and ultimately prevent NVB
public speech. Jake, the environmentalist, believed it is because of NVB that our environment is
in decline and Ben said we must learn to talk about difficult issues with SVB.
In spite of the fact that humans
have language, our spoken communication is very much like that of animals. “Perhaps
the best known examples can be found among alarm calls, which refer to
different types of predators of a given species and elicit type-specific
avoidance behavior(Diana monkeys, Cercopithecus
diana , Zuberbuhler,
Noe, & Seyfarth, 1997; suricata, Suricata
suricatta , Manser, Seyfarth, & Cheney, 2002; prairie
dog, Cynomys spp.,
Slobodchikoff, Fischer, & Shapiro, 1986; chicken, Gallus gallus,
Evans,
Evans, & Marler, 1993).” Like monkeys, we too produce “alarm calls”, that
is, we produce NVB; like prairie dogs, we too produce NVB sounds “which refer
to different types of predators”; and, like chickens, our sound too changes when
we are threatened by predators, who produce NVB. NVB is produced by those who
threaten as well as those who are threatened. NVB is the language of threat, fear,
intimidation and aggression. SVB, on the other hand, is the language of
affiliation, sociality and peace. Unfortunately, “These calls are” mistakenly “considered as
functionally referential.” Animal researchers anthropomorphize by thinking that
humans are different from animals as they have language. Although we have
language, we are, biologically speaking, more similar than different than
animals.
As we misunderstand our verbal behavior, as we believe to be causing it,
we explain animal behavior in terms of reference too. Presumably something inside of the animal, the “caller’s inner state”,
causes it to call. Consequently, ethologists wrongly continue to “emphasize
animal communication is more than simply sending signals about the caller’s
inner state: Showing the proper behavioral response to a referential signal
also requires learning from the receivers.” Let’s leave out “a referential
signal” as it doesn’t explain anything. “The proper behavioral response” only
“requires learning” or conditioning due to environmental stimuli, in other
words, behavior is selected by consequences. Another way of describing the
process, in which the caller, the speaker, learns from the receiver, the
listener, is by emphasizing the two can’t exist separate from each other.
The caller doesn’t only learn from the receiver, but the receiver also
learns from the caller. Calling and receiving are bi-directional rather than uni-directional
phenomena. These authors misrepresent the research by Owren and Rendall (1997),
which points out that this whole information processing business is wrong. The
organism-environment relationship is sufficient for explaining behavior. Their
““affect-conditioning” model for nonhuman primate (and many other animal)
vocalizations”, also accounts for human vocalizations and doesn’t involve a
self.
“The most effective signals are” NOT
“those that directly affect the receivers’ inner state”, and consequently the
behavior, but those that directly change “their behavior.” This phenomenon can
be experienced when our interaction changes from SVB to NVB. If we pay
attention to how we sound while we speak, we immediately notice that as soon as
a threat occurs this changes the sound of our voice. We are used to talking
about feelings as inner states and that is why we are so bad at expressing emotions.
As long as we continue to think emotions represent some imaginary inner state,
we have an inaccurate account of how we are immediately affected by our
environment, that is, by each other. There is a distinction between
contingency-governed and rule-governed behavior. Due to language we overestimate
the importance of rule-governed behavior, behavior that is function of verbal
instruction, and underestimate the importance of contingency-governed behavior,
behavior that is a function of other people.
Not surprisingly, these researchers don’t go into Owren’s
& Rendall’s (1997) “Affect-Conditioning Model.” They mention it only in
passing, but they don’t and can’t acknowledge the importance of this model for human
interaction. Similar to primates, during human interaction “responses of receivers
can be unconditioned, when the response is being produced by the signal itself,
and conditioned, when the receiver’s response is a result of past social
interactions between them—that is, where the caller elicited affective
responses in the receiver through other means." They mention here the
difference that behaviorist make between contingency-shaped versus
rule-governed behavior. However, they completely misrepresent the “Affect
Conditioning Model” by using it to validate research which was debunked by
Owren and Rendall. The whole issue of “referentiality” is bogus.
Nothing can stop these authors from writing “This latter approach
[Affect Induction Model], especially in nonhuman animal species, is not far
from the definition of functional referentiality and gives further support for
those opinions, which argue beside the dual (affective/referential)
nature of many of the animal signal” (e.g.,Seyfarth& Cheney, 2003). Owren
and Rendall wouldn’t give any support to this definition of “functional
referentiality,” which is based on the debunked “Information
Processing Model”. This is a classic example of what is described in
entry-level psychology books as confirmation bias, “the
tendency to search for, interpret, favor, and recall information in a way that
confirms one's beliefs or hypotheses, while giving disproportionately less
consideration to alternative possibilities”. They
hang on to their NVB of the “referential/affect-conditioning paradigm.”